.

Wednesday, January 23, 2019

Key Success Factors for Online Advertising

friendly advertize Catherine Tucker? February 15, 2012 Abstract In br separately publicizing, ads atomic image 18 fooled base on be assistantly net profits and their national is custom-built with propoundation that pertains to the favorable relationship. This paper explores the e? electroconvulsive therapyiveness of tender advertizing using development from ? long time tests of di? erent ads on Facebook. We ? nd try out that friendly advert is e? ective, and that this e? cacy depends to stem main(prenominal)ly from the ability of orienting ground on favorable cyberspaces to unc e realplace similarly responsive consumers.However, fond publicize is slight(prenominal) e? ective if the adman self-explanatoryly states they atomic number 18 trying to promote friendly in? uence in the text of their ad. This purposes that advertisers must avoid universe unfastened in their assays to exploit companionable networks in their announce. Catherine Tucker is Associate prof of merchandising at MIT Sloan School of Man come onment, Cambridge, MA. and Faculty Research Fellow at the NBER. Thank-you to Google for ? nancial support and to an anonymous non-pro? t for their cooperation.Thank-you to Jon Baker, Ann Kronrod, Preston Mcafee, and seminar participants at the George Mason University Roundtable on the Law and Economics of Internet Search, the University of Rochester, UCLA and Wharton for valuable comments. every last(predicate) errors be my own. ? 1 Electronic copy available at http//ssrn. com/ regard=1975897 1 opening Recent advances on the internet develop every last(predicate)owed consumers to move crosswise digital favorable networks. This is pickings place at unprecedented levels Facebook was the most visited website in the US in 2010, accounting for 20% of full-length time exhausted on the internet, a higher counterweight than Google or hayseed ComScore, 2011). However, it is link that traditional marketing communi cations affirm been at the periphery of this explosion of well-disposed data despite the documented index number of loving in? uence on purchasing behavior. Much of the emphasis on marketing in affectionate media, so far, has been on the achievement of realize r from each one, whereby a dishonor builds its subscriber base organic totallyy and a exchangeable hopes that this volition in? uence others organically finished sharing links with their kind networks (Corcoran, 2009). However, recent enquiry by Bakshy et al. 2011) has emphasized that this kind of organic sharing is far rarer than foregoingly supposed, and that at that place are very some examples of a commercial message being legitimately transmitted across loving networks. Further, Tucker (2011a) delegates that in secernate to achieve virality, an advertiser may have to sacri? ce the commercial e? ectiveness of their message. This essence that advertisers may need to wasting disease give publicise to help oneself the sharing of their commercial message through favorable networks. Both Facebook and LinkedIn have recently introduced a new form of advertisement called amicable advertizing. A social ad is an online ad that incorpo pass judgment user actions that the consumer has agreed to boast and be shared. The resulting ad showings these interactions along with the users mortala (picture and/or arrive at) inwardly the ad confine (IAB, 2009). This re inserts a radical technological development for advertisers, because it heart and soul that electromotive forcely they scum bag co-opt the condition of an individuals social network to target advertisement and engage their audience. This paper asks whether social advertising is e? ective, and what active steps advertisers themselves should squeeze in their ads to promote social in? ence. 2 Electronic copy available at http//ssrn. com/abstract=1975897 We explore the e? ectiveness of social ads using data from a ? eld experiment conducted on Facebook by a non-pro? t. This ? eld experiment pard the mathematical process of social ads with customaryly targeted and untargeted ads. The social ads were targeted to the friends of buffers of the brotherly love on Facebook. The ads featured that cull kayoeds attain and the fact that they had become a fan of this kind-heartedness. We ? nd that on average these social ads were much e? ective than demographically targeted or untargeted ads.Further, this proficiency is multipurpose for improving both the death penalty of demographically targeted and untargeted plys. Comparing the performance of these ads that contained the name of the fan and were targeted towards the fans friends with those that were simply targeted to that fans friends suggests that their e? ectiveness stems predominantly from the ability of social targeting to uncover similarly responsive consumers. We vex results that suggest that as well as being more e? ective at gath ering clicks, social advertising is alike more e? ective at promoting actual subscriptions to the newsfeed and is more cost-e? ctive. We then turn to study how advertisers should forge their social advertising. Through randomized ? eld tests, we investigate the e? ectiveness of advertisers advisedly promoting social in? uence in their advertising copy through including a statement that encourages the witnesser to, for example, be like their friend. We ? nd that consumers reject attempts by advertisers to explicitly rein or boot to a friends actions in their ad copy. This result subscriber lines with introductory semi confirmable research that ? nds consistent bene? ts to ? rms from highlighting previous consumer actions to positively in? ence the consumers response (Algesheimer et al. , 2010 Tucker and Zhang, 2011). This rejection is middling uniform across di? erent wording, though slightly less(prenominal) severe for ads that mode arrange a less explicit reference to friendship. We then present superfluous establish to regularize turn up two potential definitions for our ? ndings. First, we rule out that the overt mention of social in? uence simply made mass aware they were visual perception an ad alternatively than something organic to the site. We do this by comparing an ad that states it is an ad with an ad that does non, and ? nding no di? rence. 3 Second, to investigate whether it was simply bad advertising copy, we examined how the ads perform for a crowd of Facebook users who have shown a visible propensity for social in? uence. We identify such(prenominal)(prenominal) users by whether or not they have a stated adhesiveness to a agency Brand on their Facebook pro? le. These users, in contrast to our earlier results, play off more positively to the advertiser explicitly co-opting social in? uence than to a message that did not. This suggests that it was not simply that the message was staidly communicated, but instead re? cts a taste (or more accu rovely distaste) for explicit references to social in? uence among most, though not all, consumers. This research builds on a lit that has studied the interplay surrounded by social networks and word of mouth. Zubcsek and Sarvary (2011) present a theoretical representative that examines the e? ects of advertising to a social network, but assume that a ? rm cannot directly use the social network for marketing purposes. Instead, ? rms have to imprecate on consumers to organically pass their advertising message within the social networks. There has been little work on advertising in social networks.Previous studies in marketing about social network sites have hesitancyed how such sites can use advertising to chance members (Trusov et al. , 2009), and alike how makers of applications designed to be used on social network sites can best advertise their products (Aral and Walker, 2011) through viral marketing. Hill et al. (2006) show that phone communications data can be used to call in who is more likely to adopt a service, Bagherjeiran et al. (2010) present a unimaginative application where they use data from instant messaging logs at bumpkin to improve online advertising targeting, and similarly Provost et al. 2009) show how to use browsing data to match root words of users who are socially similar. Tucker (2011b) explores how privacy controls mediate the e? ectiveness of advertising on Facebook. However, to our knowledge this is the ? rst academic study of the e? ectiveness of social advertising. Managerially, our results have important implications. companionable advertising and the use of online social networks is e? ective. However, when advertisers attempt to strengthen this social 4 in? uence in ad copy, consumers come on less likely to resolve positively to the ad. This is, to our knowledge, the ? st piece of empirical support for emerging managerial theories that emphasize the need for ? rms to not appear too obviously commercial when exploiting social media (Gossieaux and Moran, 2010). 5 2 work Experiment The ? eld experiment was impress by a bittie non-pro? t that provides bringing upal scholarships for girls to attend high school in easternmost Africa. Without the intervention of this non-pro? t, and other non-pro? ts like them, girls do not attend lowly school because their families prioritize the education of sons. though the non-pro? ts main mission is funding these educational scholarships, the non-pro? has a secondary mission which is to inform young state in the US about the state of education for African girls. It was in aid of this secondary mission that the non-pro? t notice up a Facebook page. This page help oneselfs as a repository of interviews with girls where they guide the challenges they have faced. To launch the ? eld experiment, the non-pro? t followed the procedure described in A/B Testing your Facebook Ads Getting give away results through experimentation (Faceb ook, 2010) which involved setting up ten-fold competing shake ups. These ad shake ups was targeted to three di? erent groups as shown in prorogue 1. The ? st group was a broad untargeted campaign for all Facebook users aged 18 and older in the US. The second group were state who had already expressed interest in other charities. These populate were identi? ed using Facebooks broad category targeting of brotherly love + Causes. The third group were tribe who had already expressed an interest in Education + Teaching. Previously, the pilot ladder had tried such reasonably broad targeting with little success and was anticipant that social advertising would improve the ads performance (Tucker, 2011b). In all cases, the charity explicitly excluded current fans from seeing its ads.For each of these groups of Facebook users, the non-pro? t launched a socially targeted nisus. These ads employed the Facebook ad option that meant that they were targeted only to users who were fri ends of existing fans of the charity. This also meant that when the fan had not opted-out on Facebook, the ad also displayed a social sanction where the name of the friend was shown at the bottom of the ad as shown in Figure 1. 6 fudge 1 Di? erent Groups Targeted Condition Untargeted baseline lonesome(prenominal) Shown Baseline text All volume in US over age of 18 who are not fans of the non-pro? t already.All people in US over age of 18 who state a? nity with charities on their Facebook pro? le who are not fans of the non-pro? t already. All people in US over age of 18 who state a? nity with education on their Facebook pro? le who are not fans of the non-pro? t already. societal Variant Shown all 5 texts from set back 2 All people in US over age of 18 who are friends of the non-pro? ts supporters who are not fans of the non-pro? t already. All people in US over age of 18 who state a? nity with charities on their Facebook pro? le who are friends of the non-pro? ts supporters who are not fans of the nonpro? already. All people in US over age of 18 who state a? nity with education on their Facebook pro? le who are friends of the non-pro? ts supporters who are not fans of the nonpro? t already. Charity Education The non-pro? t varied whether the campaign was demographically targeted and whether the campaign was socially targeted, and also explored di? erent ad-text conditions. Table 2 describes the di? erent ad-copy for each condition. Each di? erent type of ad-copy was accompanied by the same picture of an appealing secondary-school student who had bene? ted from their program.The socially targeted ads displayed all ? ve chance changeables of the advertising message depicted in Table 2. For each of the non-socially-targeted campaigns, we ran the baseline variant of the ad text which, as shown in Table 2, simply says answer girls in easternmost Africa change their lives through education. The non-pro? t could not run the other four conditions that ref er to others actions, because federal regulations require ads to be dependable and they did not want to mis tow potential supporters. The di? erent ad conditions were broadly designed to cover the kinds of normative and informational social in? ence described by Deutsch and Gerard (1955) Burnkrant and Cousineau (1975). 1 We want to be all the way that we do not argue that these advertising measures 1 Other forms of social in? uence studied in the literature involve network externalities where there is a performance bene? t to multiple people adopting (Tucker, 2008). However, that does not seem to be relevant 7 Table 2 Di? erent Ad-Text Conditions Condition Baseline Be like your friend Ad-Text friend girls in East Africa change their lives through education. Be like your friend.Help girls in East Africa change their lives through education. gullt be left out. Help girls in East Africa change their lives through education. Your friend knows this is a good cause. Help girls in Ea st Africa change their lives through education. hit the books from your friend. Help girls in East Africa change their lives through education. Dont be left out. Your friend knows Learn from your friend. capture all types of social in? uence or are necessarily successful at distinguishing amongst the di? erent types of social in? uence that are possible. The literature on social in? ence has emphasized that the underlying mechanism is nuanced and complex. Obviously, di? erent types of social in? uence relate and interact in moods that cannot be teased apart simply with di? erent wording. However, the adaptation in messages does allow us to study whether explicit advertising messages that attempt to use di? erent types of wording to evoke social in? uence are e? ective in general. Figure 1 Sample Ad Figure 1 displays an anonymized sample ad for a social ad in the be like your friend condition. The blacked-out top of the ad contained the non-pro? ts name. The grayedhere. out botto m of the ad contained a supporters name, who had liked the charity and was a Facebook friend of the person who was being announce to. It is only with developments in technology and the development of automated algorithms that such individualized display of the friends name when pertinent is possible. Table 3 describes the demographics of the roughly 1,500 fans at the beginning of the campaign. Though the initial fans were reasonably spread out across di? erent age cohorts, they were more female than the average population, which makes sense given the personality of the charity.At the end of the experiment, the fans were slightly more likely to be male than before. The way that Facebook broods data means that we have access to the demographics only of the fans of the charity, not of those who were advertised to. Table 3 presentationgraphics of the non-pro? ts fans before and after the ? eld experiment Age 18-24 25-34 35-44 45-54 55+ Total Before Male 5 5 6 3 3 22 Experiment afte rward Experiment Female Male Female 13 8 14 14 6 14 17 6 16 13 3 13 10 4 10 67 27 67 The Total row does not add up to one C% because fans who are below 18 years of age are omitted. 9 3 DataThe data that Facebook shares with advertisers is both anonymous and aggregate. This means that we cannot trace the e? ects of social advertising on the friends of every one individual. It also means that we cannot examine heterogeneity in the degrees of in? uence across individuals, as is studied, for example, by Godes and Mayzlin (2009) in their study of o? ine ? rm-sponsored communications. However, given that the central research question of the study is whether, on average, di? erent types of social advertising are more e? ective, the aggregate nature of the data is su? cient.Table 4 ideas daily abstract statistics for the campaigns in our data. Over a 5-week period, there were 630 observations. There were 18 campaigns in supply that consisted of a) The three baseline conditions that were demographically targeted to everyone, charity-lovers and education-supporters and used the baseline text, and b) The ? fteen social ad conditions that had all the ? ve di? erent types of text, and socially targeted sepa layly to everyone, charity-lovers and education-supporters. Table A2 in the appendix provides a summary of these campaigns. Table 4 Summary Statistics Mean Std Dev Min Max just Impressions 13815. 13898. 6 1 98037 Average pokeys 5. 06 5. 17 0 37 Connections 2. 70 3. 52 0 24 Unique gaols 5. 04 5. 14 0 36 Daily Click valuate 0. 11 0. 10 0 1. 27 Impression Click vagabond 0. 045 0. 047 0 0. 50 Cost Per Click (USD) 0. 98 0. 40 0. 31 3. 90 Cost Per 1000 views (USD) 0. 52 1. 37 0 24. 5 Ad-Reach 6165. 7 6185. 0 1 60981 frequence 2. 32 0. 82 1 9. 70 18 ad variants at the daily level for 5 weeks (630 observations) There are two click-through rates reported in Table 4. The ? rst click-through rate is the proportion of people who clicked on an ad that twenty-four hours. T he denominator here is the 10Ad-Reach measure that captures the number of people exposed to an ad each day. The second click-through rate is per ad impression. We focus on the condition in our econometric analysis, because impressions can be a function of person refreshing their page or using the back button on the browser or other actions which do not necessarily get out to increased exposure to the ad. We show cogency ulteriorly to using this click-through rate per impression measure. Due to the relatively small number of clicks, these click through rates are expressed as serving foretells or sometimes as fractions of a percentage drive.In our regression analysis we also use this scaling in order to make our coe? cients more easily readable. 2 The data also contains an resource means of measuring advertising success. The radio link rate measures the number of people who liked a Facebook page within 24 hours of seeing a sponsored ad, where the denominator is the ads reach that day. We compare this measure to clicks in subsequent analysis to invert that the click-through rate is capturing something meaningful. We also use the cost data about how much the advertiser paid for each of these ads in a robustness check.The data reassuringly suggests that there were only ? ve occasions where someone clicked twice on the ads. Therefore, 99. 8% of the click-through rate we measure captures a single individual clicking on the ad. 2 11 Figure 2 Social advertising is e? ective 4 4. 1 Results Does Social publicise Work? First, we present some simple evidence about whether social advertising is more e? ective than regular display advertising. Figure 2 displays the basic equality of aggregate (that is, across the whole ? ve-week period) click-through rates between non-socially-targeted ads and ads that were socially targeted.Since these are aggregate click-through rates they di? er from the daily click-through rates reported in Table 4. These are expressed as fra ctions of a percentage point. It is trim that social advertising earned far vauntingly-scaler click-through rates. The di? erence between the two bars is quite striking. To check the robustness and statistical signi? cance of this relationship, we turn to econometrics. The econometric analysis is relatively unequivocal because of the randomization induced by the ? eld tests. We model the click-through rate of campaign j on day t targeted to demographic group k as 2 ClickRatejt = ? SocialT argeting arcsecondj + ? k + ? t + j (1) SocialT argeting mugj is an index for whether or not this campaign variance was socially targeted and displayed the stock warrant. Since Facebook does not allow the test of these di? erent features separately, this is a combined (rather than separable) indicator. ?k is a ? xed e? ect that captures whether this was the untargeted variant of the ad. This controls for underlying systematic di? erences in how likely people within that target and untarget ed segment were to respond to this charity.We include a vector of troth dummies ? t . Because the ads are randomized, ? t and ? k should primarily improve e? ciency. We picture the speci? cation using ordinary least squares. Though we recognize that theoretically a click-through rate is bounded at one hundred since it is measured in percentage points, click-through rates in our data are never cosy to this upper bound or lower bound. 3 Table 5 reports our initial results. chromatography column (1) presents results for the simple speci? cation implied by equation (1) but without the date and demographic controls.The point estimates suggest that social targeting and a friends imprimatur increased the average daily clickthrough rate by nearly half. Column (2) repeats the analysis with the controls for date. It suggests that after controlling for date, the result holds. This is reassuring and suggests that any un eveness in how ads were served across days does not drive our results . It also suggests that our result is not an artifact of a failure of randomization. Column (3) adds an tautological coe? cient that indicates whether that campaign was untargeted rather than being targeted to one of the customer groups identi? d as being likely targets by the non-pro? t We also tried alternative speci? cations where we use the unbounded clicks measure (rather than a rate) as the dependent variable and show that our results are robust to such a speci? cation in Table A1, in the appendix. 3 13 Educational and Charity supporters. It suggests that indeed, as expected, an untargeted campaign was weakly ine? ective, though the estimate is not signi? pitch at conventional levels. We speculate that the apparent weakness of demographic targeting may be because target markets of charity and educational supporters is reasonably broad, and consequently may have ontained many an(prenominal) individuals who would not support an international charity. An obvious question is w hat explains the success of social advertising. One explanation is that the blurb of a friend is informative. Another explanation is that social targeting uncovers people who will be more likely to be interested in their charity as they are similar, in unobserved slipway, to their friends who are already fans of the charity. Manski (1993) pointed out that this particular issue of distinguishing homophily (unobserved characteristics that make friends carry on in a similar way) from the explicit in? ence of friends on each other is empirically problematic. Ideally, to address this we would simply randomize whether users saw the s or not. However, Facebooks advertiser interface does not allow that. What we can do is take advantage of the fact that sometimes ads are shown to people without the endorsement if that fan has selected a privacy setting which restricts the use of their forecast and name. The interface which users use to do this is displayed in Figure A1 all users do is simply select the No One rather than the Only my friends option.Of melt, this will not represent perfect randomization. It is likely that the fans who select stricter privacy settings di? er in unobserved ways from those who do not, and that therefore their social networks may di? er as well. However, despite this potential for bias, this does represent a useful opportunity to try to disentangle the power of social targeting to enable homophily and the power of personal endorsements. Column (4) displays the results of a speci? cation for equation (1) where the dependent variable is the revolution rate for these socially targeted but not socially endorsed ads.Here for ads that were being shown to friends, the click-through rate was only calculated for occasions when the endorsement was not shown. A comparison of Column 14 (3) and Column (4) in Table 5 makes it clear the ads that were displayed to friends of fans but lacked a clear endorsement were less e? ective than those that had a clear endorsement. However, they were still measurably more e? ective than non-socially-targeted ads. It appears that, roughly, the endorsement accounted for less than half of the persuasive e? ect and the ability to use social networks to target the ad accounted for slightly more than half of such ads e? acy. Columns (5) and (6) of Table 5 estimate the speci? cation separately by whether the campaign was targeted or untargeted. Though the point estimate for the targeted campaigns is higher, it is notable that social advertising modify the performance of both targeted and untargeted campaigns. Given the widely reported lack of e? cacy of untargeted campaigns (Reiley and Lewis, 2009), the increase in e? ectiveness allowed by social advertising appears large for untargeted campaigns. 15 Table 5 Social Targeting and arcsecond is E? ective (4) No Endorsement Click Rate SocialTargeting EndorsementAll (1) Click Rate 0. 0386??? (0. 0123) (2) Click Rate 0. 0385??? (0. 0108) 0. 0287?? ( 0. 0143) -0. 000275 (0. 0122) 0. 0794??? (0. 0116) 0. 0132 (0. 0166) (3) Click Rate 0. 0386??? (0. 0125) Untargeted (5) Click Rate 0. 0297??? (0. 00755) Targeted (6) Click Rate 0. 0376??? (0. 00927) SocialTargeting Untargeted ceaseless 16 battle Controls No Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Observations 630 630 630 630 210 420 Log-Likelihood 542. 1 610. 3 610. 3 427. 8 187. 7 452. 3 R-Squared 0. 0221 0. 212 0. 212 0. 119 0. 317 0. 228 OLS Estimates. low-level variable is the percentage point of people who click on the ad.Dependent variable in Columns (4) for social ads is the percentage point daily click-through rate of ads that did not display the endorsement. Robust mensuration errors. * p < 0. 10, ** p < 0. 05, *** p < 0. 01 4. 2 Robustness Table 6 checks the robustness of the ? nding that social targeting and endorsement are effective, to di? erent de? nitions of the dependent variable. Column (1) reports the results of using a dependent measure which is the percentage click-through per impression. Again, we ? nd that social advertising is more e? ective, though the e? ectiveness is less pronounced and less on the button estimated than before.This suggests that the appeal of social advertising is not necessarily enhanced by multiple exposure. It could also, of course, merely re? ect noise introduced into the process by someone refreshing their browser multiple times. The results so far suggest that consumer privacy concerns or the intrusiveness of such ads do not seem to outweigh the appeal of social advertising for consumers. 4 There is forever and a day the possibility of course that people clicked on the ads because they were annoyed or wanted to understand more the extent of privacy intrusion rather than because the ads were rattling e? ective.To explore this, we estimate a speci? cation where the dependent measure was the proportion of clicks that became subscribers of the newsfeed. The results are reported in Column (2). We see that again social adve rtising appears to be more e? ective at encouraging Facebook users to take the intended action as well as simply clicking. This is evidence that people are not clicking on social ads due to botheration at their intrusiveness but instead are clicking on them and taking the action the ads intend to encourage them to take. Untargeted ads are less likely to lead to conversions than those targeted at appropriate demographics.This makes sense these people are being targeted just because they are the kind of people who have signed up for such news feeds in the past. A ? nal question is whether ads that are socially targeted and display endorsements are more expensive for advertisers, thereby wiping out their relative e? ectiveness in terms of return on advertising investment. We explore this in Column (3) of Table 6. There are This may be because Facebook users ? nd it reassuring that these ads, though narrowly targeted, are not overly visually intrusive (Goldfarb and Tucker, 2011). 4 17 everal missing observations where there were no clicks that day and consequently there was no price recorded. In Column (3), we report the results of a speci? cation where our explanatory variables is the relative price per click. The results suggest that advertisers earnings less for these clicks that are socially targeted. This suggests that Facebook is not charging a premium for this kind of advertising. Though Facebook shrouds in secrecy the precise pricing and auction mechanism underlying their advertising pricing, this result would be consistent with a mechanism whereby advertisers pay less for clicks if they have higher clickthrough rates.In other words, prices paid bene? t from an modify quality-score (Athey and Nekipelov, 2011). The results also suggest that advertisers pay less for demographically untargeted clicks which is in line with previous studies such as Beales (2010). Table 6 Social ad is E? ective Checking robustness to di? erent dependent variables SocialTarg eting Endorsement (1) Click Rate (Multiple) 0. 0108?? (0. 00501) 0. 00526 (0. 00582) Yes 630 1086. 5 0. cl (2) Clicks to Connections Rate 0. 433??? (0. 0997) -0. 321??? (0. 0768) Yes 554 -467. 5 0. 163 (3) Cost Per Click (USD) -0. 95??? (0. 0480) -0. 177??? (0. 0520) Yes 559 -129. 0 0. 426 Untargeted go out Controls Observations Log-Likelihood R-Squared OLS Estimates. Dependent variable is the click-through rate (expressed as a fraction of a percentage point) for impressions in Column (1). Dependent variable in Column (2) is the clicks to conversions rate. Dependent variable in Column (3) is cost per click. Robust mensuration errors. * p < 0. 10, ** p < 0. 05, *** p < 0. 01 4. 3 What Kind of Social publicise gists Work? We then go on to explore what kind of advertising message works in social ads.We distinguish between ads that rely simply on the Facebook algorithm to promote social in? uence by featuring the automated endorsement at the bottom of their ad, and ads that ex plicitly refer to this endorsement in their ad copy. 18 Table 7 Social Advertising is Less E? ective if an Advertiser is Too Explicit (3) No Endorsement Click Rate SocialTargeting Endorsement All (1) Click Rate 0. 0577??? (0. 0139) (2) Click Rate 0. 0571??? (0. 0113) 0. 0333?? (0. 0168) -0. 0287??? (0. 00886) -0. 000463 (0. 0122) -0. 0136 (0. 0115) -0. 0189? (0. 01000) -0. 0378??? (0. 0115) -0. 0429??? (0. 0144) -0. 101 (0. 0124) Yes 630 615. 4 0. 225 Yes 630 618. 1 0. 232 Yes 630 429. 5 0. 124 Yes 210 189. 6 0. 329 Yes 420 461. 0 0. 260 -0. 000281 (0. 0177) 0. 0161 (0. 0169) -0. 0303? (0. 0167) -0. 0284?? (0. 0124) Untargeted (4) Click Rate 0. 0498?? (0. 0245) Targeted (5) Click Rate 0. 0527??? (0. 0130) SocialTargeting SocialTargeting Endorsement ? Explicit Untargeted SocialTargeting Endorsement ? Dont be left out SocialTargeting Endorsement ? Be like your friend SocialTargeting Endorsement ? Learn from your friend 19 SocialTargeting Endorsement ? Your friend knows SocialTargeting ? ExplicitDate Controls Observations Log-Likelihood R-Squared OLS Estimates. Dependent variable is the percentage points of people who click on the ad. Dependent variable in Columns (3) adjusted for social ads so that is the percentage point daily click-through rate of ads that did not display the endorsement. Robust touchstone errors. * p < 0. 10, ** p < 0. 05, *** p < 0. 01 We use the additional binary indicator variable Explicitj to indicate when the advertiser uses a message that evokes social in? uence explicitly in their ad copy, in addition to the social endorsement automated by the Facebook algorithm.This covers all the non-baseline conditions described in Table 2. We interact this with the SocialT argeting Endorsementj , meaning that SocialT argeting Endorsementj now measures the e? ect of the baseline effect, and the interacted variable measures the incremental advantage or disadvantage of mentioning the friend or the potential for social in? uence in the ad. Colum n (1) of Table 7 reports the results. The negative coe? cient on the interaction between Explicit and SocialT argeting Endorsementj suggests that explicit reference to a social in? uence mechanism in the ad a? ected the performance of the ad negatively.That is, when the advertiser themselves were explicit about their intention to harness social in? uence, it back? res. Further, the large point estimate for SocialT argeting Endorsementj suggests that the baseline message is even more e? ective than the estimates of Table 5 suggested. Column (2) in Table 7 reports the results of a speci? cation where we break up Explicit by the di? erent types of social in? uence- center advertising messages featured in Table 2. It is striking that all measures are negative. It is also suggestive that the one message that was not statistically signi? ant and had a smaller point estimate than the others did not refer to the friend explicitly but instead referred obliquely to the friends action. This is speculative, since the point estimate here is not statistically di? erent from the others due to its large standard error. Column (3) repeats the exercise for the click-through rate for the ads that did not display an endorsement that we investigated in Table 5. Since these ads did not display the friends name at the bottom, it should not be so obvious to a viewer that the ? rm is explicitly trying to harness the social in? uence that results from the friend being a fan of the charity.We recognize that there may of course be some confusion at the mention of a friend when no name is displayed, but this confusion should work against us rather than for us. In this case, 20 we do not see a negative and signi? cant e? ect of the Explicit advertising message which referred to a friend. This suggests that it was the faction of the friends name and the mention of social in? uence which was oddly o? -putting. The results in Column (3) suggest that what is damaging is the combination of an advertiser making it explicit they are trying to harness social in? ence and the algorithmic social advertising message. We next explored whether this ? nding that attempts by advertisers to explicitly harness social in? uence in their ad text damaged the e? ectiveness of social advertising di? ered by the target group selected. Column (4) presents the results for the campaign that was targeted at friends of fans who were simply over 18 years old and based in the US. Column (5) presents the results for the group of users whom the charity selected as being in the target demographic groups for the campaign that is users whose Facebook pro? e revealed their support for other educational and charitable causes. What is striking is the similarity of the estimates for the e? cacy of social advertising and the damage do by the advertiser being overly explicit about social in? uence across Columns (4) and (5). Again, similar to the results reported in Table 5 social advertising appears to be able to o? er as nearly as large a emanation to ad e? cacy for an untargeted population as a targeted one. 4. 4 behavioral Mechanism We then collected additional data to help rule out alternative explanations of our ? nding that the explicit mention of social in? ence was undesirable in social ads. One obvious potential explanation is that what we are measuring is simply that people are unaware that what they are seeing is actually an ad, rather than part of Facebook. When a non-pro? t uses a message such as Be like your friend then it becomes obvious that this is an ad, and people respond di? erently. To test this, we persuaded the non-pro? t to run a subsequent experiment that allowed us to explicitly tease this apart. In this experiment we compared the performance of ads that said Please read this ad. Help girls in East Africa 21 change their lives through education. , and ads that simply said Help girls in East Africa change their lives through education. 5 If it is was t he case that Facebook users were simply mistaking socially targeted ads for regular content and the explicit appeals to social in? uence stopped them making this mistake, we would expect to also see a negative e? ect of wording that made it clear that the message was an ad. However, it appears that adding Please read this ad if anything helped ad performance, which suggests that it was not the case that Facebook users were simply mistaking socially targeted ads for content if there is no explicit message.Obviously, though, the sample size here is very small, making more de? nitive pronouncements unwise. Table 8 Not Driven by Lack of Awareness of Advertising or Universally Unappealing Ad Copy Knowledge (1) Click Rate 0. 0312? (0. 0160) 0. 0114 (0. 0288) appearance (2) Click Rate 0. 0194 (0. 0208) 0. 0376? (0. 0221) 0. 0449? (0. 0254) -0. 00448 (0. 0218) 0. 0172 (0. 0254) 0. 127?? (0. 0584) (3) Click Rate 0. 0182 (0. 0208) SocialTargeting Endorsement SocialTargeting Endorsement ? Exp licit SocialTargeting Endorsement ? Dont be left out SocialTargeting Endorsement Be like your friend SocialTargeting Endorsement ? Learn from your friend SocialTargeting Endorsement ? Your friend knows Date Controls Yes Yes Yes Observations 20 60 60 Log-Likelihood 55. 43 91. 77 103. 7 R-Squared 0. 916 0. 267 0. 508 OLS Estimates. Dependent variable is the percentage point of people who click on ad that day. Robust standard errors. * p < 0. 10, ** p < 0. 05, *** p < 0. 01 Recent research has questioned the use of the imperative in advertising copy, which is why we used delight (Kronrod et al. , 2012) 5 22 Another alternative explanation for our ? dings is that the messages referring to the friend were poorly-written or unappealing. To test whether this was the case, we selected an alternative set of users whom might be expected to react in an opposite way to potential presumptions of social in? uence. Speci? cally, the charity agreed to run test conditions identical to those i n Table 2 for the people who expressed a? nity with Fashion goods on their Facebook pro? les. The Fashion category of users were chosen because typical models of social in? uence have focused on fashion cycles (Bikhchandani et al. , 1992).These models emphasize the extent to which people who participate in Fashion cycles receive explicit utility from conformity, even when this conformity is arouse by a ? rm. In other words, they may ? nd advertiser-endorsed social in? uence more persuasive and advertiser attempts at emphasizing the power of social in? uence more acceptable than the general population does. This group of users exhibits a very di? erent pattern to that exhibited by the general population. They appear to respond somewhat positively to social advertising, though this estimate is imprecise and the point estimate is smaller than for the other conditions.However, strikingly, they reacted particularly positively to advertising messages that emphasized social in? uence and the actions of the friend in the ad copy. In other words, social advertising for this group worked even when the advertiser explicitly embraced the potential for social in? uence. This result suggests that there may be heterogeneity in consumer responses to the wording of social advertising messages depending on their previous economic consumption patterns. This is evidence against an alternative explanation for our results in Table 7 based on these advertising messages which explicitly refer to the potential for social in? ence being confusing or overly wordy, since they were e? ective for this group of Fashion fans. In general, the results of Tables 7 and 8 suggest that there is heterogeneity in distaste for advertiser attempts to harness social in? uence given previous consumption patterns, but that for the average person the e? ects are negative. 23 5 Implications How helpful is data on social relationships when it comes to targeting and delivering advertising content? This pap er answers this question using ? eld test data of di? erent ads on the large social network site Facebook. We ? nd evidence that social advertising is indeed very e? ctive. This is important, as for the past few years social network websites have often been dismissed by advertisers as venues for paid media, that is, paid advertising. Instead, the emphasis was on earned or organic media whereby social networks were venues for organic word of mouth. This dismissal of paid advertisements was echoed in the popular and marketing press with headlines such as Online Social vane and Advertising Dont Mix and Facebook Ad Click-Through Rates atomic number 18 Really Pitiful (Joel, 2008 Barefoot and Szabo, 2008). Our results suggest, however, that as social advertising develops this will change swiftly.In particular, social networks will be able to exploit their respectable inherent network e? ects to enlarge their share of advertising dollars. Strikingly, we ? nd that the average Facebook use r appears to ? nd social advertising as done by the standard Facebook algorithm appealing. However, when advertisers attempt to emulate or reinforce this social in? uence, consumers appear less likely to respond positively to the ad. Speculatively, the results suggest that intrusive or highly personal advertising is more acceptable if done algorithmically by a faceless entity uch as a computer than when it is the result of evident human agency. Very speculatively, there is perhaps a parallel with users of web-based email programs accepting an algorithm scanning their emails to serve them relevant ads when the interception of emails by a human agent would not be acceptable. Our results suggest that social advertising works well for both targeted and untargeted populations, which may mean that social advertising is a particularly useful technique when 24 advertising to consumers outside the products natural or obvious market segment since their are less obvious ways of targeting in t hese settings.The majority of this e? cacy appears to be because social targeting uncovers unobserved homophily between users of a website and their underlying receptiveness to an advertising message. There are of course limitations to our study. First, the non-pro? t setting may bias our results in ways that we cannot predict. Second, the aims of the non-pro? t also means the outcome measure we study is whether or not people sign up to hear more about the nonpro? t, rather than studying the direct e? ect of advertising on for-pro? t outcomes such as customers making purchases.Third, we studied this advertising at a time when Facebook was just launching and promoting its social advertising features. It is not clear whether the results will be as strong if the advertising market becomes vestal with social ads. Notwithstanding these limitations, we believe that this paper makes a useful section in terms of documenting when social advertising is useful and when it is not. 25 Referenc es Algesheimer, R. , S. Borle, U. M. Dholakia, and S. S. Singh (July/ disdainful 2010). The impact of customer community participation on customer behaviors An empirical investigation.Marketing Science 29 (4), 756769. Aral, S. and D. Walker (September 2011). Creating social contagion through viral product design A randomized trial of peer in? uence in networks. Management Science 57 (9), 16231639. Athey, S. and D. Nekipelov (2011). A structural model of sponsored search advertising auctions. Mimeo, Berkeley. Bagherjeiran, A. , R. P. Bhatt, R. Parekh, and V. Chaoji (2010). Online advertising in social networks. In B. Furht (Ed. ), enchiridion of Social Network Technologies and Applications, pp. 651689. Springer US. Bakshy, E. , J. M. Hofman, W. A. Mason, and D. J. Watts (2011). Everyones an in? encer quantifying in? uence on Twitter. In Proceedings of the fourth ACM international conference on sack up search and data mining, WSDM 11, unseasoned York, NY, USA, pp. 6574. ACM. Barefoo t, D. and J. Szabo (2008, April). Facebook ad click-through rates are really pitiful. Friends with Bene? ts A social media handbook blog. Beales, H. (2010). The value of behavioral targeting. Mimeo, George capital of the United States University. Bikhchandani, S. , D. Hirshleifer, and I. Welch (1992, October). A theory of fads, fashion, custom, and cultural change in informational cascades. Journal of political Economy 100 (5), 9921026. 6 Burnkrant, R. E. and A. Cousineau (1975, December). Informational and normative social in? uence in buyer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research 2 (3), 20615. ComScore (2011). ComScores 2011 social report Facebook leading, microblogging growing, world connecting. White Paper . Corcoran, S. (2009, December 16). De? ning earned, owned and paid media. Forrester Research. Deutsch, M. and H. B. Gerard (1955). A study of normative and informational social in? uences upon individual judgment. The Journal of atypical and Social Psychology 51 (3), 629636. Facebook (2010, September).A/B testing your Facebook ads Getting better results through experimentation. Technical Report. Godes, D. and D. Mayzlin (2009). Firm-Created Word-of-Mouth Communication Evidence from a Field Test. Marketing Science 28 (4), 721739. Goldfarb, A. and C. Tucker (2011, May). Online display advertising Targeting and obtrusiveness. Marketing Science 30, 389404. Gossieaux, F. and E. Moran (2010). The Hyper-Social Organization Eclipse Your Competition by leverage Social Media. McGraw-Hill. Hill, S. , F. Provest, and C. Volinksky (2006). Network-based marketing Identifying likely adopters via consumer networks.Statistical Science 21 (2), 256276. IAB (2009). IAB social advertising best practices. Interactive Advertising Bureau. Joel, M. (2008, February 8). Online social networks and advertising dont mix. Six Pixels of Separation, Twist Image. 27 Kronrod, A. , A. Grinstein, and L. Wathieu (2012, January). Go green Should environmental messages be so assertive? Journa l of Marketing 76, 95102. Manski, C. F. (1993, July). Identi? cation of endogenous social e? ects The re? ection problem. Review of Economic Studies 60 (3), 53142. Provost, F. , B. Dalessandro, R. Hook, X. Zhang, and A. Murray (2009).Audience selection for online brand advertising privacy-friendly social network targeting. In Proceedings of the fifteenth ACM SIGKDD international conference on Knowledge discovery and data mining, KDD 09, New York, NY, USA, pp. 707716. ACM. Reiley, D. and R. Lewis (2009). Retail advertising works measuring the e? ects of advertising on sales via a controlled experiment on Yahoo . Working Paper, Yahoo Research. Trusov, M. , R. E. Bucklin, and K. Pauwels (2009, September). E? ects of word-of-mouth versus traditional marketing Findings from an internet social networking site.Journal of Marketing 73, 90102. Tucker, C. (2008). Identifying formal and informal in? uence in technology word meaning with network externalities. Management Science 54 (12), 2024 2038. Tucker, C. (2011a). Ad virality and ad persuasiveness. Mimeo, MIT . Tucker, C. (2011b). Mimeo, MIT . Tucker, C. and J. Zhang (2011). How does popularity information a? ect choices? A ? eld experiment. Management Science 57 (5), 828842. Zubcsek, P. and M. Sarvary (2011). Advertising to a social network. Quantitative Marketing and Economics 9, 71107. Social Networks, Personalized Advertising, and Privacy Controls. 28Figure A1 Control interface for switching o? Endorsement A-1 Table A1 Robustness of Table 5 to using number of clicks as dependent variable OLS (1) Average Clicks SocialTargeting Endorsement 1. 991??? (0. 394) -0. 0385 (0. 422) 0. 000405??? (0. 0000443) Poisson (2) Average Clicks 0. 258??? (0. 0746) 0. 134 (0. 0817) 0. 0000327??? (0. 00000638) veto Binomial (3) Average Clicks 0. 230?? (0. 0922) 0. 187 (0. 123) 0. 0000455??? (0. 0000135) Untargeted Ad-Reach Date Controls Yes Yes Yes Observations 630 630 630 Log-Likelihood -1484. 8 -1417. 6 -1394. 7 R-Squared 0. 755 O LS Estimates in Columns (1)-(2).Dependent variable is the Number of clicks on the ad in Columns (3)-(4). Robust standard errors. * p < 0. 10, ** p < 0. 05, *** p < 0. 01 A-2 Table A2 Summary of 18 discharges Campaign 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 Social Ad? Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Social Advertising Non-Social Advertising Non-Social Advertising Non-Social Advertising monstrance Targeting?Demo 1 Targeted Demo 1 Targeted Demo 1 Targeted Demo 1 Targeted Demo 1 Targeted Demo 2 Targeted Demo 2 Targeted Demo 2 Targeted Demo 2 Targeted Demo 2 Targeted Untargeted Untargeted Untargeted Untargeted Untargeted Demo 1 Targeted Demo 2 Targeted Untargeted Message Baseline Message 1 Message 2 Message 3 Message 4 Baseline Message 1 Message 2 Message 3 Message 4 Baseline Message 1 Message 2 Message 3 Message 4 Baseline Baseline Baseline A-3

No comments:

Post a Comment